PSYCHOLOGY

are the ones that give us this particular pain sensation.

(a) When heat is being conducted away from the skin we usually experience an

increase in .

(b) What skin sense is induced by tissue damage?

Answers: (a) cold; (b) Pain.

Smell: The Nose Knows

You may think to yourself, "Someone in this room is wearing a perfume that I

can't stand!" How do you know? You can't see the perfume. You can't hear the

perfume. But you, with your sense of smell, know.

The sense of smell allows us to detect the presence of some, but not all, airborne

chemical substances. The sense of smell is also known as olfaction. The

52 PSYCHOLOGY

receptor organ that makes the sense of smell possible is called the olfactory

epithelium, and it is located high in the nose. It is to smell what the retina is to

vision. Several kinds of neurons differentially sensitive to chemicals in gaseous

forms induce the various smell sensations.

The exact number and kind of basic smell sensations, unlike the four basic

taste sensations, are somewhat debatable. Nonetheless, it is possible to identify a

number of elemental sensations. Putrid is one of them; it is the smell of something

rotting or decomposing. Blossoms and blooms have a floral odor. A smell that is

sharp or acrid, such as that produced by burning food, is said to be pungent. Cinnamon

or cloves are said to have a smell that is spicy. The wood and bark of the

camphor tree have a penetrating, fragrant odor. Camphor is also obtained by synthesis

and is used in some medicines. The odor is described simply as camphoric.

(a) The sense of smell is also known as .

(b) The receptor organ that makes smell possible is called the .

Answers: (a) olfaction; (b) olfactory epithelium.

Kinesthesis: Can You Touch the Tip of Your Nose

with Your Eyes Closed?

If you can touch the tip of your nose with your eyes closed, as most people can, you

have an intact sense of kinesthesis. Kinesthesis, also known as proprioception,

is the capacity to know the position in space of various parts of your body. (The

term proprioception is related to the word "property." Your body belongs to you—

it's your property.) Close your eyes and lift or lower a single finger. You know

where it is at all times. When you walk you can sense the position of your legs even

if you're not looking at them. Pianists and dancers rely heavily on kinesthesis.

The receptor neurons for kinesthesis are located in the connective tissue surrounding

the body's joints as well as within the joints themselves.

(a) Kinesthesis is also known as .

(b) Kinesthesis is the capacity to know the position in space of .

Answers: (a) proprioception; (b) various parts of your body.

The Sense of Balance:Walking in an Upright Position

The sense of balance informs you that you are walking in an upright position. What

you are sensing is the relationship of your body, and in particular your head, to the

Earth's gravitational field. The sense of balance is made possible by receptor neurons

Sensation: Studying the Gateways of Experience 53

located in the semicircular canals. Located in the inner ear, the canals are tubular

bones filled with fluid. The movement of this fluid stimulates the firing of receptor

neurons within the canals, and the information is transmitted to the brain.

The sense of balance is also known as the vestibular sense. A vestibule is a

small antechamber or passageway. This is one way to describe the semicircular

canals, important components of the apparatus involved in the sense of balance.

(a) The sense of balance is also known as the .

(b) The sense of balance is made possible by receptor organs located in the .

Answers: (a) vestibular sense; (b) semicircular canals.

SELF-TEST

1. Sensation refers to

a. organized experience

b. thinking and concept formation

c. meaningful knowledge

d. the raw data of experience

2. The waves to which we give the name "light" are a narrow band of

a. the electromagnetic spectrum

b. radio waves

c. ultra-violet waves

d. infra-red waves

3. The trichromatic theory proposes that we have

a. three kinds of optic nerves

b. a triad of lenses

c. three kinds of cones

d. three kinds of rods

4. A sound wave

a. can travel through outer space

b. cannot travel through water

c. has frequency, but not amplitude

d. requires a medium such as air or water

5. What structure in the ear is similar in function to the eye's retina?

a. The basilar membrane

b. The auditory nerve

c. The tympanic membrane

d. The oval window

54 PSYCHOLOGY

6. The four basic taste sensations are

a. sweet, salty, bitter, and hot

b. sweet, bitter, burned, and salty

c. sour, acid, sweet, and mint

d. sweet, salty, bitter, and sour

7. One of the following is not a skin sense.

a. Light touch

b. Incongruent pleasure

c. Deep touch

d. Temperature

8. The receptor organ that makes smell possible is called the

a. vestibular membrane

b. olfactory epithelium

c. odor membrane

d. synaptic epithelium

9. What sense makes it possible for you to touch the tip of your nose with your

eyes closed?

a. The vestibular sense

b. The cardiovascular sense

c. Kinesthesis

d. Synthesis

10. The vestibular sense lets you know when

a. a signal is present

b. a figure is perceived against a ground

c. you are walking upright

d. you have a subliminal perception

ANSWERS TO THE SELF-TEST

1-d 2-a 3-c 4-d 5-a 6-d 7-b 8-b 9-c 10-c

ANSWERS TO THE TRUE-OR-FALSE PREVIEW QUIZ

1. True.

2. True.

3. False. An electromagnetic wave, not a sound wave, has the remarkable property of

being able to travel through a vacuum.

4. True.

Sensation: Studying the Gateways of Experience 55

5. False. Receptor organs in both the connective tissue surrounding the body's joints as

well as within the joints themselves make possible the sense of kinesthesis, the capacity

to know the position in space of various parts of your body.

KEY TERMS

56 PSYCHOLOGY

auditory nerve

basilar membrane

brightness

cochlea

cognition

cones

cornea

decibel (dB)

deep touch

electromagnetic spectrum

electromagnetic wave

fovea

hertz (Hz)

hue

incus

infra-red rays

kinesthesis

lens

light touch

loudness

malleus

olfaction

olfactory epithelium

optic nerve

oval window

pain

perception

periphery

photons

photoreceptors

pitch

proprioception

retina

rods

saturation

semicircular canals

sensation

skin senses

stapes

taste buds

temperature

timbre

trichromatic theory

tympanic membrane

ultra-violet rays

vestibular sense

vestibule

5 Perception: Why Do Things

Look the Way They Do?

57

PREVIEW QUIZ

True or False

1. T F In the study of perception, a distinction can be made between the

geographical world and the psychological world.

2. T F Figure-ground perception is always stable and never reversible.

3. T F Research suggests that there are innate, or inborn, organizing tendencies

in perception.

4. T F Learning appears to play no part in perception.

5. T F Clairvoyance is another name for psychokinesis.

(Answers can be found on page 70.)

The link between sensation and perception is clear. Perception is possible

because we have sensations. The raw data of experience—sensations—

become organized wholes at the level of perception. We experience a world

of objects—trees and songs—not flashes of light and random bits of sound.

In this chapter you will learn how this organization arises.

Objectives

After completing this chapter, you will be able to

• state the Gestalt laws of perception;

• describe the role that learning plays in perception;

• explain what illusions teach us about perception;

• explain how both binocular vision and monocular cues play a role in depth perception;

• discuss some of issues associated with the topic of extrasensory perception.

Kurt Koffka (1886–1941), one of the founders of Gestalt psychology, said that

the great question of perception is: "Why do things look the way they do?"

At first the question seems almost silly. We are tempted to answer, "Because

things are they way they are." It would seem that tall things look tall because they

are tall. And distant things look distant because they are distant. On the other

hand, why does the Moon look larger just above the horizon than it does when

it's overhead? It hasn't gotten any bigger, or any closer. And, if a series of disconnected

dots are arranged in the pattern of, say, the letter F, it looks like the letter,

not a bunch of disconnected dots—which, it could be argued, it actually is.

You learned in the last chapter that visual images on your retina are upsidedown.

Nonetheless, you perceive them as right side up. At the level of sensation, it's

an inverted world. At the level of perception, the world doesn't look inverted at all.

Koffka's question does not have to be limited to the sense of vision. The same

question could be adapted to the other senses. The principles set forth in this

chapter, largely in connection with vision, can be readily applied to perception in

general.

Sensation, as indicated in chapter 4, is the raw data of experience. Perception,

on the other hand, is the organization and the meaning we give to primitive

information. It can be said with some degree of confidence that we use

sensory information to create a psychological world.

Returning to Koffka, he said that there is a distinction between the geographical

world and the psychological world. The geographical world is the

actual world "out there," the world as defined and described by physics. The psychological

world is the world "in here," the world as experienced by the subject.

Although common sense usually says it's the so-called "real world" or

physical world that determines our behavior, it can be argued that common sense

isn't sufficiently analytical. Reflection suggests that we behave in terms of what

we perceive to be true, not necessarily in terms of what is actually true.

If ice is thin in the physical world, and it is solid in your psychological world, you

are likely to skate on it. And, of course, you may make a serious mistake as a result.

58 PSYCHOLOGY

In sum, it can be argued that we act to a large extent in terms of our perceptions.

And it is for this reason that the study of perception is a basic one in psychology.

(a) Perception is the and the meaning we give to primitive information.

(b) Koffka made a distinction between the geographical world and the world.

Answers: (a) organization; (b) psychological.

The Gestalt Laws: Is Our Perception of the World

Due to Inborn Organizing Tendencies?

Imagine that you are looking up and you see a single bird flying in the sky. The

bird is a figure, a well-defined perceptual object tending to stand out. The sky is

ground (or background), the perceptual field that surrounds the figure. This is

figure-ground perception. One of the features of this kind of perception is that

the figure is usually smaller than the ground and tends to be seen as coming forward

from the ground. Other examples include seeing a button on a blouse, a

book on a table, or a car on the road.

It can be argued that this kind of perception, the ability to distinguish a figure

from a field, is an inborn organizing tendency. We aren't taught to do it. We probably

start doing it spontaneously early in infancy. An infant reaching for a milk

bottle suggests to us that he or she perceives the bottle as a perceptual object, a figure

in a field. Figure-ground perception is probably the most fundamental organizing

tendency we possess.

Keep in mind once again that perception does not necessarily reflect the structure

of the world itself. For example, a word printed in black ink on a white page

is perceived as slightly in front of the white surface. We are tempted to think that

this is because the word is "on" the page. But imagine that a black piece of paper

is covered with a stencil. The entire page is inked white, with the exception of the

word. Now, from a physical point of view, the white ink is on the black surface.

Nonetheless, unless carefully studied, the word, emerging in black, will be perceived

as slightly forward and on the page.

perception is probably the most fundamental organizing tendency we

possess.